Is the Chinese language a superstition machine?

Every year, more than a billion people around the world celebrate Chinese New Year and engage in a subtle linguistic dance with luck.

You can think of it as a set of holiday rituals that resemble a courtship.

To lure good fortune into their lives, they may decorate their homes and doors with paper cutouts of lucky words or phrases.

Those who need a haircut make sure to get one before the New Year, as the word for “hair” (fa) sounds like the word for “prosperity”—and who wants to snip away prosperity, even if it’s just a trim?

The menu of food served at festive meals often includes fish, because its name (yu) sounds the same as the word for “surplus”; a type of algae known as fat choy because in Cantonese it sounds like “get rich”; and oranges, because in certain regions their name sounds like the word for “luck.”

Fishing4luck.jpg

Fish flags are displayed at the Changdian New Year Temple Fair in Beijing

English speakers can relish a good pun, and messing around with homophones (words that sound the same but have different meanings) is a staple of many a clever ad.

But Chinese practices take punning to a whole new level—one that reaches deep into a culture where good fortune is persistently courted through positive words and deeds, and misfortune repelled by banishing the negative.

The number four is tainted because of its homophony with the word for “death”—many Chinese people would never consider buying a house whose address contained that number.

In visual designs, fish and bats figure prominently because they are sound twins of the words for “surplus” and “fortune.”

Gift-giving is fraught with homophonic taboos; it is all right to give apples, because their name sounds like “peace” but not pears, whose name overlaps with “separation.”

Questions about why certain objects or numbers are considered lucky or unlucky are often met with matter-of-fact statements about their sound-alikes.

But why is homophony deeply woven into Chinese traditions and symbols but not Western ones?

Ambiguity and homophony are facts of life for all languages, on a scale that rarely enters the consciousness of their native speakers.

In 1978 psycholinguist Bruce Britton sifted through a million-word body of English text and estimated—very conservatively—that at least 32 percent of English words have more than one meaning.

Among the 100 most frequent words, he found that 93 percent have more than one meaning, some as many as 30.

Linguist blogger Geoff Pullum captures this ambiguity with his question:

“What do support poles, staff positions, battery terminals, army encampments, blog articles, earring stems, trading stations, and snail mail have in common with billboard advertising, accounts recording, making bail, and assigning diplomats?”

It may take a few moments of pondering to realize that the word that makes strange bedfellows of all these notions is post.

English speakers are sanguine about homophony—often blithely, so that they make little attempt to clarify meanings even when the context leaves open the possibility of more than one.

In one study led by Victor Ferreira, people were asked to describe objects in visual scenes that showed both a baseball bat and a flying bat—but they ambiguously referred to either of these as simply “the bat,” under some conditions as often as 63 percent of the time.

But Chinese speakers seem to have a more sensitive radar for ambiguity.

Psycholinguists Michael Yip and Eiling Yee have shared with me their impressions that Chinese speakers are more likely to take pains to clarify the intended meaning of an ambiguous word, even when its meaning should be obvious from the context.

For example, Yee reported that they might say the equivalent of:

“I have to renew my mortgage, so I have an appointment with my bank—you know, the financial institution, not a river bank.”

This type of attunement to ambiguity, if it turns out to be a general characteristic of Chinese speakers, certainly meshes with the elevated role of homophony in Chinese culture.

The connections between language, mind, and culture represent an open vista, a terrain that, for the most part, still awaits exploration by scientists.

But, even today, the dance between ambiguity and luck in Chinese cultural practices hints at tantalizing questions:

Do different languages heighten different subjective experiences for their speakers?

And does the importance of certain concepts—like luck and misfortune—to a culture leave an imprint on its associated language?

Pullum contends that languages make no attempt whatsoever to skirt ambiguity—on the contrary, he writes:

“Languages love multiple meanings.

They lust after them.

They roll around in them like a dog in fresh grass.”

Steve Piantadosi, a psycholinguist at the University of Rochester, agrees.

He and his colleagues have argued that far from being a bug, ambiguity is a useful feature for languages.

It allows them to create ample vocabularies by recycling some of their most common and easy-to-pronounce clumps of sound.

Without ambiguity, we would have to create longer words to distinguish meanings, or become more inventive at coming up with massive collections of speech sounds—and more adept at pronouncing and distinguishing these sounds.

FortunateFruit.jpg

Because the word for “orange” sounds like the word for “luck,” Chinese New Year decorations often include illustrations of citrus fruits

If all languages love ambiguity, then Chinese languages burn with ardor for it.

Direct comparisons of rates of ambiguity between languages are tricky, because even within a single language, different dictionaries often disagree about the number of separate meanings they assign to a word.

But by any measure, Chinese homophony is truly prolific.

In English, as in many other languages, basic units of meaning (called morphemes) are often composed of processions of multiple syllables—hippopotamus, president, fastidious.

But in languages like Cantonese and Mandarin, morphemes are almost always monosyllabic.

These monosyllables aren’t necessarily stand-alone words, as most words in Chinese are compounds made up of two or more morphemes, each of which is represented by a separate character.

Still, each syllable has to be assigned its correct meaning in order for a Chinese listener to grasp the intended meaning of the compound.

Add to that the fact that Chinese languages use a much smaller set of vowels and consonants than English, and you have an impressive number of meanings packed into a small sliver of phonetic real estate.

Since distinct meanings in Cantonese or Mandarin are typically expressed by different characters, orthography (the way a language transfers spoken words into written form) is one useful way to track the number of different meanings that have the same pronunciation.

Researchers Li Hai Tan and Charles Perfetti reported that within a 1.8-million-character body of Chinese text, 4,500 characters mapped onto only 420 different syllables—so that each character shared its pronunciation with an average of 11 others.

In Piantadosi’s dataset, even if we look only at monosyllabic English words, which are more ambiguous than multisyllabic words, the average number of homophones that each word has is slightly less than one.

Amazingly, there’s no reason to believe that Chinese ambiguity creates a communication problem—experiments show that Chinese speakers are just as efficient as English speakers at squashing down irrelevant meanings based on context, and differences in tone also help to distinguish meanings.

But the design of the Chinese writing system, in which different meanings of the same syllable are expressed by different characters, makes it hard for Chinese speakers to ignore the fact that they’re swimming in a sea of ambiguity. 

Ambiguity has a way of drawing attention to itself when it creates orthographic headaches.

While the many meanings of post may never have ruffled your thoughts, chances are that at one time or another, you’ve probably devoted a fair bit of focused effort to the distinctions among their, they’re and there as you tried to make sure that your spelling captured your intended meaning.

Imagine having to do that for every set of homophones you encounter and you get a bit of a sense of what it takes to become literate in Chinese.

No wonder then, that among Chinese children who are learning to read, a poor awareness of homophones is one of the strongest predictors of reading problems such as dyslexia.

By JULIE SEDIVY

Source: Nautilus

 

Advertisements

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out /  Change )

Google+ photo

You are commenting using your Google+ account. Log Out /  Change )

Twitter picture

You are commenting using your Twitter account. Log Out /  Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out /  Change )

Connecting to %s